Unidentified bystanders in warzones are seen as guilty until proven innocent

Publicly released:
New Zealand; International
USAF Thunderbirds Fly-by, image by UX Gun via UnSplash
USAF Thunderbirds Fly-by, image by UX Gun via UnSplash

People tend to assume that unidentified bystanders in war are enemy combatants, according to a new paper by New Zealand and UK researchers. Researchers presented over 2000 participants with a moral dilemma where they had to decide whether a pilot should bomb a dangerous enemy target that would also kill a bystander. About 1 in 4 Americans supported a military strike that would kill a civilian, but 53% said they would endorse a strike if the bystander was "unidentified."  Bombing endorsement was lower overall for UK participants, however in both the UK and the US, people who saw civilians as acceptable or desirable military targets were more likely to assume that the bystander was a enemy combatant.

Media release

From: PLOS

Bystanders in a combat zone are treated as guilty until proven innocent

People assume unidentified bystanders in a warzone are combatants and may therefore be acceptable collateral damage.

People’s bias toward sacrificing unknown bystanders appears to stem from assuming the unidentified person is an enemy, according to a study published October 23, 2024, in the open-access journal PLOS ONE by Scott Danielson from the University of Canterbury, New Zealand, and colleagues.

About as many civilians as soldiers die in war each year, some during strikes targeted at enemy combatants. There have been many reported cases of mistaking innocent civilians for enemy combatants, with the possibility of many more being unreported.

Danielson and colleagues conducted five experiments to test when people assume unknown bystanders in a combat zone are enemies rather than civilians, reducing their concerns about collateral damage. A total of 2,204 participants were presented with a realistic moral dilemma: A military pilot must decide whether to bomb a dangerous enemy target, also killing a bystander.

The first two experiments used as the scenario a conflict between the U.S. and ISIS in Iraq. Here, few people endorsed bombing when the bystander was known to be an innocent civilian. However, when the bystander’s identity was unknown, more than twice as many people endorsed the bombing. Crucially, approximately half of American participants endorsed sacrificing unidentified bystanders despite no evidence they were enemies -- a sacrificial rate higher than any identified target except a known enemy combatant. The subsequent three experiments used a fictional war setting and revealed a similar pattern of judgment was seen here too.

Bombing endorsement was predicted by attitudes toward total war: the theory that there should be no distinction between military and civilian targets in wartime conflict. The authors also noted key differences between the participant samples based on their country - bombing endorsement was lower for participants in the U.K. compared to the U.S., which the researchers attribute to national and cultural differences in total war attitudes.

According to the authors, these findings have implications for military strategists who must decide whether to attack areas with enemy militants and unidentified bystanders. The results support a common tendency in people to assume the bystanders are enemies, with important consequences if they turn out to be innocent civilians. The real-world cases of civilians struck by bombs could result from the same error in judgment reported in this study. To minimize civilian deaths, future research should investigate how to reduce this bias and convince decision-makers to evaluate more carefully who their weapons are targeting.

The authors add: “Fewer than 1 in 4 Americans support a military strike that kills a civilian, but 53% endorse a strike if the bystander is "unidentified." Data from five studies show that people are more likely to assume unknown bystanders are enemy combatants than civilians, which might explain the high civilian death toll in modern warfare.”

Expert Reaction

These comments have been collated by the Science Media Centre to provide a variety of expert perspectives on this issue. Feel free to use these quotes in your stories. Views expressed are the personal opinions of the experts named. They do not represent the views of the SMC or any other organisation unless specifically stated.

Dr Andrew Vonasch, Senior Lecturer Above the Bar School of Psychology, Speech and Hearing, University of Canterbury, comments:

Our research shows that people have a tendency to assume unidentified bystanders in war are enemy combatants. As a result, unknown persons are treated like enemies, often being considered acceptable collateral damage. Modern warfare, which increasingly relies on airstrikes, is particularly susceptible to this bias, which may play a role in the high number of civilian deaths we hear reported from conflicts around the globe.

"In a series of five experiments involving over 2,000 participants, individuals were presented with a moral dilemma: whether to bomb a dangerous enemy target, knowing it would also kill a civilian bystander. The results showed that only 24% of participants were willing to endorse a bombing when the bystander was identified as an innocent civilian. However, when the bystander’s identity was unknown, 53% participants supported the bombing, more than double the rate.

Last updated:  24 Oct 2024 10:05am
Contact information
Contact details are only visible to registered journalists.
Declared conflicts of interest None declared.

Multimedia

Percent of “Yes” responses to “Should the pilot fire?”
Percent of “Yes” responses to “Should the pilot fire?”

Attachments

Note: Not all attachments are visible to the general public. Research URLs will go live after the embargo ends.

Research PLOS, Web page Press-only preview
Research PLOS, Web page Link will go live after the embargo lifts
Journal/
conference:
PLOS ONE
Research:Paper
Organisation/s: University of Canterbury, University of Southhampton
Funder: The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.
Media Contact/s
Contact details are only visible to registered journalists.