Aussies and cockatoos locked in rubbish arms race

Publicly released:
Australia; International; NSW; ACT
Credit: Barbara Klump / Current Biology
Credit: Barbara Klump / Current Biology

We love to throw out rubbish while sulphur-crested cockatoos love to eat it, and now Australian and European scientists have detailed the techniques used by both people and parrots to achieve these disparate aims. The team noted the birds typically pry open bins with their beaks, manoeuvre themselves onto a small rim, and flip the lid open, a technique they can learn from other cockatoos. Unfortunately for us, we can't simply lock our bins, because they need to be emptied. So, instead, people resort to putting bricks and stones on the lids, strapping water bottles to the top, rigging ropes to prevent the lid from flipping, using sticks to block the hinges, and switching tactics when the cunning cockatoos figure them out. Interestingly, just as the cockatoos learn their techniques from one another, human tactics are also passed between family, friends and neighbours. The researchers say they'd like to study the birds' seasonal bin diving habits next, but do not weigh in on who they think will be the ultimate victor in the war of the bins!

Media release

From:

In Australia, cockatoos and humans are in an arms race over garbage access

Residents of southern Sydney, Australia have been in a long-term battle over garbage—humans want to throw it out, and cockatoos want to eat it. The sulphur-crested cockatoos that call the area home have a knack for getting into garbage bins, and people have been using inventive devices to keep them out. Researchers detail the techniques used by both people and parrots in a study publishing on September 12 in the journal Current Biology.

“When I first saw a video of the cockatoos opening the bins I thought it was such an interesting and unique behavior and I knew we needed to look into it,” says lead author Barbara Klump (@DrBarbaraKlump), a behavioral ecologist at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior.

The cockatoos’ motivation is food waste. “They really like bread,” she says. “Once one gets a bin open all the cockatoos in the vicinity will come and try to get something nice to eat.”

The birds typically pry the bins open with their beaks and then maneuver themselves onto a small rim and flip the lid open. It’s a community affair. “We could actually show that this is a cultural trait,” says Klump. “The cockatoos learn the behavior from observing other cockatoos and within each group they sort of have their own special technique, so across a wide geographic range the techniques are more dissimilar.”

Human residents trying to keep the cockatoos out can’t simply secure the bin lids completely closed because the lids need to open when tipped by an automated arm on the garbage truck. A survey given by the researchers found that people put bricks and stones on their bin lids, strap water bottles to the top, rig ropes to prevent the lid from flipping, use sticks to block the hinges, and switch tactics once the cockatoos figure them out. “There are even commercially available cockatoo locks for bins,” says Klump.

“It's not just a social learning on the cockatoo side, but it's also social learning on the human side,” she says. “People come up with new protection methods on their own, but a lot of people actually learn it from their neighbors or people on their street, so they get their inspiration from someone else.”

Klump won’t say who she expects to win the race for control of the bins, but she and her colleagues plan to look at how the cockatoos’ behavior varies from season to season.

Klump expects we will see more of these kinds of human-wildlife interactions in the future. “As cities expand, we will have more interactions with wildlife,” she says. “I'm hoping that there will be a better understanding and more tolerance for the animals that we share our lives with.”

Attachments

Note: Not all attachments are visible to the general public. Research URLs will go live after the embargo ends.

Research Cell Press, Web page The URL will go live after the embargo ends
Journal/
conference:
Current Biology
Research:Paper
Organisation/s: Australian Museum, The Australian National University, Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, Germany
Funder: This study was funded through a Max Planck Society Group Leader Fellowship to LMA, and a National Geographic Early Career Grant to BCK. LMA was further supported by the Swiss State Secretariat for Education, Research and Innovation (SERI) European Research Council (ERC) replacement scheme, under contract number MB22.00056. DRF was funded by a grant from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement No. 850859), and an Eccellenza Professorship Grant of the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant Number PCEFP3_187058).
Media Contact/s
Contact details are only visible to registered journalists.